The brain is capable of holding and retrieving1(取回,恢复) memories for specific fears, revealing a more sophisticated storage and recall capacity than previously2 thought, neuroscientists have found. The study, which appears in the journal Nature Neuroscience, may have implications(含义,启示) for treating post-traumatic stress syndrome3(综合症) —as scientists begin to understand how different fears are stored in the brain, they can move toward addressing specific fear memories. The research was conducted by researchers at New York University's Center for Neural4 Science, the Department of Psychiatry5 at NYU School of Medicine–Bellevue Hospital Center, the Copernicus Center for Interdisciplinary(跨学科研究) Studies in Krakow, Poland, Université Paris-Sud, and the Emotional Brain Institute at the Nathan S. Kline Institute for Psychiatric Research.
The research focused on the brain's amygdala(杏仁核,扁桃体) , which has previously been shown to store fear memories. However, prior studies have indicated that the amygdala does not discriminate6(区别,辨别) among the different threats it holds and processes. In other words, whether you are afraid of dogs because you were once bitten by a dog or you are afraid of pizza because you once nearly choked to death eating it, all the amygdala remembers is that both of these experiences were scary. By contrast, other brain areas, such as cortex(皮质,树皮) , ensures that all other aspects of these fearful events in your life are remembered.
The scientists on the Nature Neuroscience study sought to determine if there were differences in how the amygdala processes and remembers fears. To do so, they focused on a process called memory consolidation7(巩固,合并) in which an experience is captured, or encoded, then stored. Once consolidation occurs, memories may be long lasting—one experience may create memories that last a lifetime. However, whenever recalled, memories become labile—that is, susceptible8 to(易受……影响的) changes. This process is called reconsolidation(再压密,重新巩固) . In life, reconsolidation allows updating existing memories. But this process also serves as a valuable methodological(方法的) tool as it lets researchers control the modification9 of memories.
When it comes to developing fear memories, one model posits10 that during a fear experience, a neutral stimulus11 (e.g., a musical passage) becomes associated with a negative encounter (e.g., a dog bite). Therefore, future occurrences of this neutral stimulus, or conditioned stimulus (CS), forewarns the onset12 of the negative encounter, or unconditioned stimulus (US). Previous research shows that the association between a CS and a US is processed and stored in the amygdala.
To replicate13 this process, the researchers devised an experiment using laboratory rats. In it, they paired two distinct audio tones, which served as the neutral stimulus, or conditioned stimulus (CS), with mild electric shocks to different parts of the rats' bodies. As a result, the rats linked a mild shock to a certain part of their bodies with a certain tone.
Under the memory reconsolidation model, exposing an organism to any aspect of the learned experience brings this memory back to mind and makes it susceptible to changes. Thus, if two distinct tones were each paired with two distinct electric shocks and if the amygdala does not discriminate among different threats, then re-exposing a rat to any of these shocks should cause lability14 of all fear memories stored in the amygdala.
However, the Nature Neuroscience study yielded quite different results. The researchers found that re-exposing a rat to a particular shock (that is, one applied15 to a certain part of the body), followed by an injection of an antibiotic16(抗生素) known to disrupt reconsolidation processes, impaired17 only these associations that were linked to this particular shock. Despite the disruption of one type of fear memory, rats were still able to express fear behavior to the tone which had been paired with a shock applied to another part of the body.
The finding demonstrates that the amygdala makes distinctions among the fear memories it holds and retrieves18.