A number of studies have shown how playing video games can lead to structural1 changes in the brain, including increasing the size of some regions, or to functional2 changes, such as activating3 the areas responsible for attention or visual-spatial skills. New research from the Universitat Oberta de Catalunya (UOC) has gone further to show how cognitive4 changes can take place even years after people stop playing.
大量研究表明,玩电子游戏会导致脑结构变化,包括增加某些区域的大小,或导致大脑产生功能性改变,如激活负责注意力或视觉空间技能的区域。加泰罗尼亚欧贝塔大学的最新研究进一步表明,即使不玩游戏好几年后,其导致的认知变化也会发生。
This is one of the conclusions from the article published in Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. The study involved 27 people between the ages of 18 and 40 with and without any kind of experience with video gaming.
"People who were
avid5 gamers before
adolescence6, despite no longer playing, performed better with the working memory tasks, which require mentally holding and manipulating information to get a result," said Marc Palaus, the author of the study.
The results show that people without experience of playing video games as a child did not benefit from improvements in processing and
inhibiting7 irrelevant8 stimuli9. Indeed, they were slower than those who had played games as children, which matched what had been seen in earlier studies.
Likewise, "people who played regularly as children performed better from the outset in processing 3D objects, although these differences were
mitigated10 after the period of training in video gaming, when both groups showed similar levels," said Palaus.
The study lasted a month and the researchers analysed participants' cognitive skills, including working memory, at three points: before starting the training in video gaming, at the end of the training, and fifteen days later. The video game used was Nintendo's Super Mario 64.
The study also included 10 sessions of transcranial magnetic
stimulation11. This is non-invasive brain stimulation through the skin that temporarily changes the brain's activity.
"It uses magnetic waves which, when
applied12 to the surface of the
skull13, are able to produce electrical currents in
underlying14 neural15 populations and modify their activity," explained Palaus.
The researchers wanted to find out if combining video gaming and this kind of stimulation would improve cognitive performance, but that didn't turn out to be the case.
"We aimed to achieve
lasting16 changes. Under normal circumstances, the effects of this stimulation can last from milliseconds to tens of minutes. We wanted to achieve improved performance of certain brain functions that lasted longer than this," said Palaus.
According to Palaus, what most video games have in common is that they involve elements that make people want to continue playing, and that they gradually get harder and present a constant challenge. "These two things are enough to make it an attractive and motivating activity, which, in turn, requires constant and intense use of our brain's resources."
"Video games are a perfect recipe for strengthening our cognitive skills, almost without our noticing," he said.