While on vacation in distant locales, people often find that time moves quite differently than in the places they’re used to. In the tropics, we settle into the grooves1 of "island time" and relax thanks to a more leisurely2 rhythm. A trip to a big city can leave us exhilarated but also drained by the energetic whir of life there.
当人们去远方度假时,往往会发现时间过得与平时很不一样。热带地区悠闲的节奏让我们习惯于“岛上的时间”,过得更加放松。去大城市令人兴奋,却也被充满活力的快节奏生活弄得筋疲力尽。
The different paces of different communities also seem to be connected to other cultural characteristics. Robert Levine and his colleagues have studied the speed of life in cities around the world and across the U.S. In a series of experiments they measured how fast solitary3 pedestrians4 in a downtown core covered a distance of 60 feet (being careful to exclude those who are obviously window shopping), timed how long it took to complete a simple commercial transaction, and recorded the accuracy of randomly5 selected clocks in the downtown business area. They found that places with a faster pace of life also had more robust6 economies (as measured by GDP per capita, average purchasing power, and average caloric intake), and that people in larger cities tended to move faster than those in less populated areas. They also found truth to the stereotype7(老套) that people move slower in hotter places.
So as you might expect, fast-moving people are associated with fast-moving economies. But does that faster life translate into greater happiness? In faster places (specifically, economically developed areas of North America, Western Europe, and Asia), people were more likely to smoke, less likely to take the time to help strangers in need, and more likely to die from coronary heart disease. Yet Levine and his colleagues found that residents in faster places tended to report feeling somewhat happier with their lives than those who lived in slower places. A city’s pace of life was indeed "significantly related" to the physical, social, and psychological well-being8 of its inhabitants.
Perhaps the higher reported rates of happiness simply reflect the fact that faster places have more robust economies. But the relationship between income and reported happiness is far from obvious. According to the "Easterlin paradox(悖论,反论)" (named after economist9 Richard Easterlin), once people have enough money to meet their basic needs, having more money is not necessarily correlated with higher self-reported happiness. Easterlin’s claims are controversial and not universally accepted; even if his theory is correct, wealthier nations might be happier overall if they address the basic needs to more of their people. In any case, the ongoing10 debate indicates that we need to tread carefully when making connections between happiness and overall economic factors.
Among individuals in a society, busyness -- or the feeling of busyness -- seems to be an important factor in well-being. That feeling of busyness -- of having a lot to do and too little time in which to do itis often associated with stress and anxiety. However in many contexts being "busy" is badge of honor: Busy parents are seen as devoted11 to their children’s well-being, the busy real estate agent must be closing lots of sales, and the busy lawyer can charge a premium12 hourly rate. In US studies, the happiest people reported that they were busy, in the sense that they had little excess time, yet did not feel rushed. Like big-city dwellers13(居民), they seemed to thrive at a faster pace.
Levine’s work raises the intriguing14 possibility that an individual’s feelings about their use of time contribute as much or more to their happiness as does economics. Now the big challenge is to find out which way the causal chain works: Does the feeling of being active, yet not rushed, contribute to happiness? Or does happiness allow people to perceive(觉察) their use of time in positive ways?